dcsimg

Behavior

provided by Animal Diversity Web

These monkeys scent mark themselves by washing their hands in their own urine and rubbing their hands on their fur. This may be a way for males to advertise their sexual maturity.

Female brown capuchin monkeys in estrus try to attract the attention of the dominant male by following him and making loud calls. A wide variety of vocalizations is known from Cebus species. These were divided into 4 categories in C. olivaceus: contacting the group, contacting individuals, ending aggressive encounters, alerting the group to the presence of danger.

Gestures and facial expressions are also widely used in communication and social grooming is an important form of contact.

Communication Channels: visual ; tactile ; acoustic ; chemical

Other Communication Modes: pheromones ; scent marks

Perception Channels: visual ; tactile ; acoustic ; chemical

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Conservation Status

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Because of its high reproductive potential and its habitat flexibility, hunting and habitat destruction have not devastated populations as severely as they have other species of primates. However, certain subspecies may be threatened, including C. a. xanthosternos and C. a. robustus from the Atlantic forests of eastern Brazil.

Cebus apella is the most commonly seen monkey in Amazonia.

US Federal List: no special status

CITES: appendix ii

IUCN Red List of Threatened Species: least concern

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Benefits

provided by Animal Diversity Web

There are no known negative affects of C. apella on humans.

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Benefits

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Cebus apella has been extensively hunted for meat. These animals are also popular pets in many parts of the world. They have been trained to perform household tasks for people with disabilities and are most commonly known as the monkeys used by itinerant "organ grinders".

Positive Impacts: pet trade ; body parts are source of valuable material; research and education

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Associations

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Brown capuchin monkeys are important predators on small animals and may disperse the seeds of some forest plants when they eat the fruit.

Cebus apella can be found in mixed species groups. They are most often found in association with squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sp.). Usually the squirrel monkeys initiate the mixed group interaction, probably in order to find food more efficiently. Squirrel monkeys follow brown capuchin monkeys to new food sources, which saves them foraging time. The association does not seem to benefit capuchin monkeys.

Ecosystem Impact: disperses seeds

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Trophic Strategy

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Fruits are a large part of brown capuchin monkey's diet. This species can eat larger fruits than other species of capuchin monkey because of their robust jaws. Vegetation, seeds, pith, eggs, insects, reptiles, birds, and small mammals (such as mouse opossums) are also included in their diet. During the dry season, when food is scarce, Scheelea palm frond pith is critical to this animal's survival.

Foraging is a noisy and destructive activity. Brown capuchin monkeys move from tree to tree, ripping apart vegetation and cracking open nuts against branches. They look for vertebrate and invertebrate prey by destroying dead vegetation and capturing anything inside. Individuals who catch prey do not usually share with other group members.

Animal Foods: birds; mammals; amphibians; reptiles; eggs; carrion ; insects; terrestrial non-insect arthropods

Plant Foods: leaves; roots and tubers; wood, bark, or stems; seeds, grains, and nuts; fruit; flowers

Primary Diet: omnivore

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Distribution

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Cebus apella is found east of the Andes from Colombia and Venezuela to Paraguay and northern Argentina.

Biogeographic Regions: neotropical (Native )

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Habitat

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Cebus apella inhabits moist subtropical or tropical forests, but has also been seen in dry forest, gallery forest, disturbed and secondary forest. In northwest Argentina these monkeys live in montane forest at an elevation of 200 to 1100m. This species prefers the understory and midcanopy of the forest, but often descends from the trees to forage and play. Cebus apella has the widest range and broadest habitat tolerances of any other Cebus species.

Range elevation: 1100 (high) m.

Habitat Regions: tropical ; terrestrial

Terrestrial Biomes: forest ; rainforest ; mountains

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Life Expectancy

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Typical longevity in the wild is unknown, but captive C. apella may live to 45 years old.

Range lifespan
Status: captivity:
45 (high) years.

Average lifespan
Status: captivity:
44.6 years.

Average lifespan
Sex: male
Status: captivity:
41.0 years.

Average lifespan
Status: wild:
40.0 years.

Average lifespan
Sex: male
Status: captivity:
45.1 years.

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Morphology

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Brown capuchin monkeys vary in color from light brown to mustard yellow to black. The shoulders and underbelly are lighter than the rest of the body. There is a patch of coarse black fur on the crown of the head, sometimes referred to as a cap. Above the ears the black hairs form tufts of fur giving the species one of its common names, tufted capuchin monkey. Facial patterns are variable among individuals, but black sideburns extending from the cap are characteristic of Cebus apella. The hands, feet, and tail are either black or dark brown.

The tail is long and prehensile. Cebus apella is the only species of capuchin monkey known to carry its tail in a tight coil.

This species is robust and stocky. Brown capuchin monkeys have a deep lower jaw and large jaw muscles to accomodate a diet of large fruits and rough vegetation. Males are 34% larger in body size and have canines 22% larger than those of females.

Range mass: 1300 to 4800 g.

Other Physical Features: endothermic ; homoiothermic; bilateral symmetry

Sexual Dimorphism: male larger

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Associations

provided by Animal Diversity Web

The dominant male protects his troop from predators by sounding alarm calls. This draws attention to himself so that his troop can escape. Capuchin monkeys are very wary of predators.

Members of a troop give sharp whistling calls at the sight of hawks and eagles, two of the most significant predators of this species. The troop is so wary of hawks and eagles that it sounds alarm whistles even if the bird flying overhead is harmless.

Known Predators:

  • hawks and eagles (Accipitridae)
license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Reproduction

provided by Animal Diversity Web

Cebus apella has a polygamous mating system. Females mate preferentially with the dominant male. Other males have mating opportunities when the dominant male is not present. During the last two days of a female's estrus cycle, the dominant male is very protective and keeps the female from mating with subordinate males.

Mating System: polygynous

Cebus apella does not appear to have a breeding season, though most births may occur during the dry season or early rainy season. The ovarian cycle lasts for approximately 21 days and female's show no external genital swellings during estrus. The gestation period is 150-160 days and one infant is born, twins are very rare.

Young males leave their natal group once they reach sexual maturity. Male brown capuchin monkeys are considered sexually mature at 7 years of age. Females mature earlier and may give birth to their first offspring as early as 4 years old.

Breeding interval: Capuchins give birth once yearly.

Breeding season: Births and breeding may occur in any season, but may be more frequent during dry or early rainy seasons.

Range number of offspring: 2 (high) .

Average number of offspring: 1.

Range gestation period: 150 to 160 days.

Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female): 4 years.

Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male): 7 years.

Key Reproductive Features: iteroparous ; year-round breeding ; gonochoric/gonochoristic/dioecious (sexes separate); sexual ; viviparous

Average birth mass: 239.7 g.

Average number of offspring: 1.5.

Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
Sex: female:
1703 days.

Parenting is left to the females, although males are tolerant of juveniles. Allomothering is a common practice. Infant capuchin monkeys cling to their mothers fur during their first few months of life. Mothers travel with their infants and nurse them during that time. If a young capuchin monkey is separated from its mother, other members of the troop will respond to the infant's distress calls. Female capuchins remain in their natal group at adulthood while males disperse.

Parental Investment: altricial ; pre-fertilization (Provisioning, Protecting: Female); pre-hatching/birth (Provisioning: Female, Protecting: Female); pre-weaning/fledging (Provisioning: Female, Protecting: Male, Female); pre-independence (Provisioning: Female, Protecting: Male, Female); post-independence association with parents; extended period of juvenile learning

license
cc-by-nc-sa-3.0
copyright
The Regents of the University of Michigan and its licensors
bibliographic citation
Anderson, R. 2003. "Cebus apella" (On-line), Animal Diversity Web. Accessed April 27, 2013 at http://animaldiversity.ummz.umich.edu/site/accounts/information/Cebus_apella.html
author
Rebecca Anderson, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
author
Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor
editor
Tanya Dewey, Animal Diversity Web
original
visit source
partner site
Animal Diversity Web

Tufted capuchin

provided by wikipedia EN

The tufted capuchin (Sapajus apella), also known as brown capuchin, black-capped capuchin, or pin monkey, is a New World primate from South America and the Caribbean islands of Trinidad and Margarita. As traditionally defined, it is one of the most widespread primates in the Neotropics, but it has recently been recommended considering the black-striped, black and golden-bellied capuchins as separate species in a new genus, thereby effectively limiting the tufted capuchin to the Amazon basin and nearby regions.[1] However, the large-headed capuchin (S. a. macrocephalus), previously defined as a distinct species, has been reclassified as a subspecies of the tufted capuchin, expanding its range east to Peru & Ecuador and south to Bolivia.[3]

The tufted capuchin is an omnivorous animal, mostly feeding on fruits and invertebrates, although it sometimes feeds on small vertebrates (e.g. lizards and bird chicks) and other plant parts. It can be found in many different kinds of environment, including moist tropical and subtropical forest, dry forest, and disturbed or secondary forest.

Like other capuchins, it is a social animal, forming groups of 8 to 15 individuals that are led by an alpha or dominant male.

Taxonomy and phylogeny

At one point all tufted capuchins were classified as Cebus apella.[4][5] Under such taxonomy, the range of C. apella would extend throughout much of South America from Colombia to northern Argentina. Although she didn't describe specific or subspecific nomenclature, Torres de Assumpção (1983; 1988) described differences between tufted capuchins from five distinct geographic regions of Brazil and high phenotypic variation of individuals in a sixth area that had a greater selection pressure.[6][7] In 2001, Silva Júnior proposed that the robust capuchins such (formerly the C. apella group) be placed in a separate genus, Sapajus, from the gracile capuchins (formerly the C. capucinus group) which retain the genus Cebus.[8] This was supported by Jessica Lynch Alfaro et al. in 2011.[9][10] Groves (2005) recognized six subspecies: Cebus apella apella, C. a. fatuellus, C. a. macrocephalus, C. a. margaritae, C. a. peruanus, C. a. tocantinus.[1] The IUCN follows Silva (2001) and recognise the species as monotypic, though the subspecies status of S. a. margaritae is unclear.[2]

Physical characteristics

Skeleton

The tufted capuchin is more powerfully built than the other capuchins, with rougher fur and a long, thick tail. It has a bundle of long, hardened hair on the forehead that can be raised as a sort of "wig". The fur is brownish gray, with the belly being somewhat lighter-colored than the rest of the body. The hands and feet are black. The tail is prehensile: strong and can be used for grasping, as an extra limb.

The tufted capuchin has a head-body length of 32 to 57 centimetres (13 to 22 in), a tail length of 38 to 56 centimetres (15 to 22 in), and a weight of 1.9 to 4.8 kilograms (4.2 to 10.6 lb), with the males generally being larger and heavier than the females.

Behaviour and ecology

Male tufted capuchin

The tufted capuchin is a diurnal, arboreal primate species, but it often forages on the ground to search for food or to walk longer distances between trees that are too far apart to jump.

The tufted capuchin lives in groups of two to twenty or more animals. A single group usually contains at least one adult male, but mixed groups with multiple males do also occur. In that case, one of the males is dominant. He accepts only a few monkeys in his direct surroundings, mainly younger animals and a few females. The dominant male and the group members that are close to him have the privilege to eat first in case of food scarcity, while subordinate monkeys have to wait until they are ready.

After a gestation period of 180 days, one young is born, or incidentally a twin. This young, which weighs only 200 to 250 grams, is carried on the back of its mother. The mother feeds her child for 9 months, but the young are sexually immature until its seventh year, which is quite late for a primate of its size.

Important natural enemies of the capuchin are large birds of prey. They are so afraid of those birds that they even become alarmed when a harmless bird flies over.

Diet

A recently discovered characteristic of one population of this species is that it uses stones as a tool to open hard nuts. First it chooses ripe nuts from a nut palm. It uses its teeth to strip off the nut's fibrous husk. Then it leaves the nut to dry for about a week. When the nut is dry, the monkey lays the nut on a large, flat rock or fallen tree,[11] hammering the nut with a suitable stone until the nut cracks. The hammerstones are often large enough to require lifting with both hands. The anvil rock is often pockmarked with hollows as a result of repeated use.[12][13]

Besides nuts, the capuchin also eats fruit, leaves, nuts,[14] seeds, pith,[15] insects and larvae, eggs and young birds, frogs, lizards, other reptiles,[15] rodents, mouse opossums,[15] and even bats. They are also known to chase cats.

The tufted capuchin looks for its food in groups. As soon as one of the group members has found something edible, he or she may make a large whistling sound, dependent upon the proximity of other individuals and abundance of the food resource so that the other monkeys know that there is something to eat.[16] The composition of the group is very well organized and is determined by rank in the hierarchy. The dominant male often resides somewhere in the middle of the group just behind the front line, so that it is safer when a predator attacks. The vanguard is composed of higher-ranked females who are tolerated by the dominant male. They have the privilege to reach the food first, but they are also the most vulnerable when a predator attacks.

Tool use and manufacture

See also: Primate archaeology

The tufted capuchin has been observed using containers to hold water, using sticks (to dig nuts, to dip for syrup, to catch ants, to reach food), using sponges to absorb juice, using stones as hammer and chisel to penetrate a barrier[17] and using stones as hammer and anvil to crack nuts.[18] While some of these tasks are relatively simple by cognitive standards (e.g. using a stick to catch ants), others, like cracking nuts with hammer and anvil are only exceeded in complexity by chimpanzees.[19]

The potential for tool use in animals like the tufted capuchin depends on a number of conditions that would increase its likelihood of appearing in a given species. Van Schaik proposed that the occurrence of tool use would be likely in foraging species if three factors were present: manual dexterity, intelligence, and social tolerance.[20] As it applies to manual dexterity, capuchins are capable of a limited precision grip (the ability to delicately pinch and manipulate objects with the thumb and fingertips), which is not found in any other New World monkeys and only found in limited amounts in apes.[21][22] C. apella has an encephalization ratio greater than the hominids (except humans) and a neocortex ratio that is almost as large as the apes; both of these rough indicators suggest high intelligence.[23][24] Finally, the tufted capuchin forms social groups typical of a complex and tolerant society.[25][26]

The tufted capuchin has been observed manufacturing tools both in captivity and in the wild. In captivity, it has been reported as making probing sticks to reach normally inaccessible containers with syrup.[27] It is also capable of understanding the concept of "sponging" and using paper towels, monkey biscuits, sticks, leaves and straw to sop up juice and then suck on the sponge to consume the juice.[27] Research in the wild has shown that capuchin tool use is every bit as extensive as in captivity with capuchins being observed using stones to dig holes to get at tubers, an activity previously only seen in humans.[28] The practice of using stones to crack nuts has arisen spontaneously in many locations such as in the Caatinga Dry Forest[28] and Serra da Capivara National Park,[29] all in Brazil and hundreds of miles apart. It has been observed cracking various nuts and fruits such as palm nuts (Attalea and Astrocaryum spp.)[30] and jatobá fruits.(Hymenaea courbaril)[29] The tufted capuchin has even been observed using stones to dislodge other stones that would later be used as hammers or shovels, an example of a more complex tool using behavior known as second-order tool use previously only found in chimpanzees.[29] Curiously, not all tufted capuchins engage in tool use. Moura and Lee (2004)[31] suggest lack of other food sources as the key factor. Ottoni and Mannu (2001),[32] Fragaszy et al. (2004)[30] and Visalberghi et al. (2005)[33] have proposed this is likely more a factor of a monkey's terrestrial habit: the more time a monkey spends on the ground, the more likely it is to profit from (and thus engage in) tool use.

In captivity, the tufted capuchin has been seen to manufacture stone tools that produced simple flakes and cores. Some of the capuchins even used these sharpened stones to cut (in a back-and-forth motion) barriers in order to reach food.[34] The importance of this behavior is that it serves as evidence of mechanical proclivity to modify stones by using behaviors already in the monkeys' repertoires, and this behavior is seen as a precursor to stone-knapping.[21] This early and limited tool use behavior has been hypothesized as similar to pre-Homo habilis and that artifacts of that time would probably resemble those of capuchins.[34]

S. apella tool manufacture and use has been analyzed for potential clues to social learning and problem solving ability, as tool manufacture and use can often shed light on such complex cognitive abilities.[17][35] Social learning, or the ability to learn from other individuals, is a controversial topic in most nonhuman species like S. apella because of the relative difficulty of determining whether a behavior was learned from imitation or a much simpler form of social learning.[36] One way of closing the gap between concurrent tool related behaviors and their likelihood of arising from imitation is by narrowing down events that would make social learning more probable such as a preference for observing experienced tool users. In this regard, Ottoni and his team found that young capuchins tended to observe the best tool users when cracking nuts.[18]

an example of a 'Doorian Fruit'
An example of the Doorian Fruit, a box that can open in one of two ways (see image for more info).

Another way of isolating imitation from other simpler behaviors is to present the capuchins with a box that has food but has two different ways of opening it. The important point is that neither way should be more advantageous so that the monkey can freely choose one. In one such study, when humans opened the door in front of the monkeys using one way only, the monkeys used that method, even when they discovered the alternative on their own.[37] In another study, capuchin alphas from two separate groups were trained to open the door in a specific way, after which the monkeys were paired with subordinates who learned to open the door in the same way.[38] When capuchins are trained in the same way and this time released into their groups, the habit is once again disseminated amongst all group members even when others discover alternative ways.[39] Nevertheless, the subject of whether or not S. apella learns by imitation is still controversial, because of the inherent difficulty in teasing out unambiguous evidence of a complex cognitive process such as imitation.[37][40]

Problem solving

Tool use and manufacture can also shed light on the many aspects of the tufted capuchin's cognitive abilities by determining how it solves some problems. Some non-primates manufacture and use objects as tools. Crows are known to make hook-tools for catching insects,[41] but such activities lack the behavioral plasticity of tool use as evidenced in tufted capuchins who found new ways to use tools that other species could not.[42] But this plasticity in tool use, while suggesting greater complexity and cognitive ability, does not suggest that the monkeys understand cause and effect. It instead implies they are only able to learn from successful efforts but not from failures, nor are they able to refine and improve much.[43] Its ability to repeat successes, coupled with its complex repertoire of behavioral events helps to explain the tufted capuchin's extensive repertoire of innovative behaviors besides tool use.[43]

Distribution and habitat

This species lives in the northern Amazon rainforest of the Guyanas, Venezuela and Brazil and to the west of the Rio Negro, as far north as the Orinoco in Venezuela. It is also found in eastern Colombia, Ecuador, Bolivia, Peru, including the upper Andean Magdalena valley in Colombia. An introduced breeding population is well established in the northwestern peninsula of the island of Trinidad in the Republic of Trinidad and Tobago. The subspecies/population on Margarita Island in Venezuela, S. a. margaritae, is considered Critically Endangered by the IUCN Red List.[44] It can be found in a large variety of forest types, mainly in tropical rainforests (up till a height of 2700 m), but also in more open forests.

The distribution overlaps with that of other species of capuchins, such as the white-fronted capuchin (Cebus albifrons).

References

  1. ^ a b c Groves, C. P. (2005). Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 137. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494.
  2. ^ a b Boubli, J.P., Stevenson, P.R., Palacios, E., de la Torre, S., Ravetta, A.L., Messias, M.R., Carvalho, A.S. & Mittermeier, R.A. 2021. Sapajus apella (amended version of 2020 assessment). The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2021: e.T172351505A192594550. https://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2021-1.RLTS.T172351505A192594550.en. Downloaded on 26 August 2021.
  3. ^ Lima, Marcela G. M.; Silva-Júnior, José de Sousa e; Černý, David; Buckner, Janet C.; Aleixo, Alexandre; Chang, Jonathan; Zheng, Jimmy; Alfaro, Michael E.; Martins, Amely; Di Fiore, Anthony; Boubli, Jean P. (1 July 2018). "A phylogenomic perspective on the robust capuchin monkey (Sapajus) radiation: First evidence for extensive population admixture across South America". Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 124: 137–150. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2018.02.023. ISSN 1055-7903. PMID 29545109. S2CID 4589398.
  4. ^ Hershkovitz, P. (1949). "Mammals of northern Colombia. Preliminary report No. 4: Monkeys (primates), with taxonomic revisions of some forms". Proceedings of the United States National Museum. 98 (3232): 1–323. doi:10.5479/si.00963801.98-3232.323.
  5. ^ Hershkovitz, P. (1955). "Notes on American monkeys of the genus Cebus". Journal of Mammalogy. 36 (3): 449–452. doi:10.2307/1375688. JSTOR 1375688.
  6. ^ Torres de Assumpcao, C. (1983). An ecological study of the primates of southeastern Brazil, with a reappraisal of Cebus apella races (PhD). University of Edinburgh.
  7. ^ Torres de Assumpcao, C. (1988). "Resultados preliminares de reavaliacao das racas do macaco-prego Cebus apella (Primates: Cebidae)". Revista Nordestina de Biologia. 6: 15–28.
  8. ^ Silva, J.S. Jr. (2001). Especiacao nos macacos-pregos e caiararas, género Cebus Erxleben, 1777 (Primates, Cebidae) (PhD). Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro.
  9. ^ Lynch Alfaro, J.W.; et al. (2011). "Explosive Pleistocene range expansion leads to widespread Amazonian sympatry between robust and gracile capuchin monkeys" (PDF). Journal of Biogeography. 39 (2): 272–288. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2699.2011.02609.x. S2CID 13791283. Archived from the original (PDF) on 26 February 2015.
  10. ^ Lynch Alfaro, J.W.; Silva, J.S. Jr.; Rylands, A.B. (2012). "How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys? An argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus". American Journal of Primatology. 74 (4): 1–14. doi:10.1002/ajp.22007. PMID 22328205. S2CID 18840598.
  11. ^ Madden, M.; Fragaszy, D.M.; Izar, P.; Hinley, A.J.; Ottoni, E.B.; Visalberghi, E. (2007). "Modelling the monkey habitat in Brazil" (PDF). Geospatial Today (5): 26–35. Archived from the original (PDF) on 17 July 2011.
  12. ^ Discovery "Life" series, "Challenges of Life" episode, David Attenborough
  13. ^ Waga, I.C.; Dacier, A.K.; Pinha, P.S.; Tavares, M.C.H. (2005). "Spontaneous tool use by wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in the Cerrado". Folia Primatologica. 77 (5): 337–344. doi:10.1159/000093698. PMID 16912501. S2CID 19546828.
  14. ^ https://sta.uwi.edu/fst/lifesciences/sites/default/files/lifesciences/images/Cebus_apella%20-%20Tufted%20Capuchin.pdf
  15. ^ a b c "Cebus apella (Brown capuchin)".
  16. ^ Di Bitetti, M.S. (2005). "Food-associated calls and audience effects in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus". Animal Behaviour. 69 (4): 911–919. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.021. S2CID 53178841.
  17. ^ a b Westergaard, G.C. (1995). "The subsistence technology of capuchins". International Journal of Primatology. 16 (1): 899–906. doi:10.1007/BF02736075. S2CID 7703148.
  18. ^ a b Ottoni, E.; Dogo de Resende, B.; Izar, P. (2005). "Watching the best nutcrackers: what capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others' tool-using skills". Animal Cognition. 8 (4): 215–219. doi:10.1007/s10071-004-0245-8. PMID 15719240. S2CID 51862020.
  19. ^ Westergaard, Gregory Charles (1999). "Structural analysis of tool-use by tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". Animal Cognition. 2 (3): 141–145. doi:10.1007/s100710050034. S2CID 40181377.
  20. ^ van Schaik, C.; D.R; M.M. (1999). "The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture" (PDF). Journal of Human Evolution. 36 (6): 719–741. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0304. PMID 10330335.
  21. ^ a b Cummins-Sebree, S.E.; Fragaszy, D.M. (2005). Capuchins as stone-knappers?: an evaluation of the evidence. Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. Cambridge, England: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research. p. 171. ISBN 978-1-902937-34-2.
  22. ^ Costello, M.; Fragaszy, D. (1988). "Prehension in Cebus and Saimiri: Grip type and hand preference". American Journal of Primatology. 15 (3): 235–245. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350150306. PMID 31968893. S2CID 86556774.
  23. ^ Rilling, J.; Insel, T.R. (August 1999). "The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging". Journal of Human Evolution. 37 (2): 191–223. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0313. PMID 10444351.
  24. ^ Dunbar, R. (1998). "The social brain hypothesis". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 6 (5): 178–190. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-8. S2CID 9555943.
  25. ^ Izar, P. (2004). "Female social relationships of Cebus apella nigritus in a southeastern Atlantic forest". Behaviour. 141 (1): 71–99. doi:10.1163/156853904772746619.
  26. ^ Visalberghi, E.; Fragaszy, D. (1995). "The behaviour of capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, with novel food: the role of social context". Animal Behaviour. 49 (4): 1089–1095. doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0137. S2CID 53191563.
  27. ^ a b Westergaard, G.C.; Fragaszy, D. (1987). "The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 101 (2): 159–168. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.101.2.159.
  28. ^ a b Moura, A.C.; Lee, PC (2004). "Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest". Science. 306 (5703): 1909. doi:10.1126/science.1102558. PMID 15591195. S2CID 36793833.
  29. ^ a b c Mannu, M.; Ottoni, E. (2009). "The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making, associative use, and secondary tools". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (3): 242–251. doi:10.1002/ajp.20642. PMID 19051323. S2CID 3479020.
  30. ^ a b Fragaszy, D.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E.; Ottoni, E.B.; de Oliveira, M.G. (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools". American Journal of Primatology. 64 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1002/ajp.20085. PMID 15580579. S2CID 16222308.
  31. ^ Moura, A.C. de A.; Lee, P.C. (2004). "Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest". Science. 306 (5703): 1909. doi:10.1126/science.1102558. PMID 15591195. S2CID 36793833.
  32. ^ Ottoni, E.B.; Mannu, M. (2001). "Semifree-ranging tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts". International Journal of Primatology. 22 (3): 347–358. doi:10.1023/A:1010747426841. S2CID 29768277.
  33. ^ Visalberghi, E.; et al. (2005). "Terrestriality and tool use". Science. 308 (5724): 951–952. doi:10.1126/science.308.5724.951c. PMID 15890860. S2CID 29819009.
  34. ^ a b Westergaard, G.C.; Suomi, S.J. (December 1995). "The stone tools of capuchins (Cebus apella)". International Journal of Primatology. 16 (6): 1017–1024. doi:10.1007/BF02696114. S2CID 33211414.
  35. ^ Visalberghi, E.; Trinca, L. (1989). "Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding". Primates. 30 (4): 511–521. doi:10.1007/BF02380877. S2CID 19285572.
  36. ^ Fragaszy, D.; E. Visalberghi (1989). "Social influences on the acquisition of tool-using behaviors in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 103 (2): 159–170. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.103.2.159. PMID 2736909.
  37. ^ a b Custance, D.; Whiten, A.; Fredman, T. (1999). "Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 113 (1): 13–22. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.13.
  38. ^ Dindo, M.; Thierry first 2=B.; Whiten, A. (2008). "Social diffusion of novel foraging methods in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 275 (1631): 187–93. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1318. PMC 2596191. PMID 17971322.
  39. ^ Dindo, M.; Whiten, A.; De Waal, F. (2009). Ferrari, P.F. (ed.). "In-group conformity sustains different foraging traditions in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". PLOS ONE. 4 (11): e7858. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.7858D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007858. PMC 2773420. PMID 19924242.
  40. ^ Visalberghi, E.; Fragaszy, D.M. (2002). "'Do monkeys ape?' - Ten years after.". In Nehaniv, C.L.; Dautenhahn, K. (eds.). Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press. p. 494. ISBN 978-0-262-04203-1.
  41. ^ Hunt, G. (1996). "Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows". Nature. 379 (6562): 249–251. Bibcode:1996Natur.379..249H. doi:10.1038/379249a0. S2CID 4352835.
  42. ^ Cummins-Sebree, S.E.; Fragaszy, D. (2005). "Choosing and using tools: Capuchins (Cebus apella) use a different metric than tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 119 (9): 210–219. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.210. PMID 15982164.
  43. ^ a b Visalberghi, E.; Limognelli, L. (1994). "Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 108 (1): 511–521. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.15. PMID 8174341.
  44. ^ Ceballos-Mago, N.; Lynch Alfaro, J.W. (2020). "Sapajus apella ssp. margaritae". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2020: e.T4079A70611951. doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-3.RLTS.T4079A70611951.en. Retrieved 18 November 2021.
license
cc-by-sa-3.0
copyright
Wikipedia authors and editors
original
visit source
partner site
wikipedia EN

Tufted capuchin: Brief Summary

provided by wikipedia EN

The tufted capuchin (Sapajus apella), also known as brown capuchin, black-capped capuchin, or pin monkey, is a New World primate from South America and the Caribbean islands of Trinidad and Margarita. As traditionally defined, it is one of the most widespread primates in the Neotropics, but it has recently been recommended considering the black-striped, black and golden-bellied capuchins as separate species in a new genus, thereby effectively limiting the tufted capuchin to the Amazon basin and nearby regions. However, the large-headed capuchin (S. a. macrocephalus), previously defined as a distinct species, has been reclassified as a subspecies of the tufted capuchin, expanding its range east to Peru & Ecuador and south to Bolivia.

The tufted capuchin is an omnivorous animal, mostly feeding on fruits and invertebrates, although it sometimes feeds on small vertebrates (e.g. lizards and bird chicks) and other plant parts. It can be found in many different kinds of environment, including moist tropical and subtropical forest, dry forest, and disturbed or secondary forest.

Like other capuchins, it is a social animal, forming groups of 8 to 15 individuals that are led by an alpha or dominant male.

license
cc-by-sa-3.0
copyright
Wikipedia authors and editors
original
visit source
partner site
wikipedia EN